How Poverty Shapes the Brain: A study co-authored by TC's Kimberly Noble offers powerful new evidence
Family income is associated with children’s brain
structure, reports a new study in Nature Neuroscience coauthored by
Teachers College faculty member Kimberly Noble.
The association appears to be strongest among children from
lower-income families.
In a sample of more than 1,000 typically developing children and
adolescents between three and 20 years old, a group led by Noble and Elizabeth
Sowell, Professor of Pediatrics at The Saban Research Institute at Children’s
Hospital Los Angeles, found that increases in both parental education and
family income were associated with increases in the surface
area of numerous brain regions, including those implicated in language
and executive functions. Family
income appeared to have a stronger, positive relationship with
brain surface area than parental education.
“We can’t say if the brain and cognitive differences we observed are causally linked to income disparities,” said Noble, who currently is both a TC Visiting Professor and Director of the Neurocognition, Early Experience and Development Lab of Columbia University Medical Center, but will join TC’s faculty as Associate Professor of Neuroscience and Education in July in the Department of Biobehavioral Sciences. “But if so, policies that target poorest families would have the largest impact on brain development.”
The results do not imply that a child’s future cognitive or brain development is predetermined by socioeconomic circumstances, the researchers said.
Noble said that among children from the lowest-income families, small differences in income were associated with relatively large differences in surface area in a number of regions of the brain associated with skills important for academic success. Conversely, among children from higher-income families, incremental increases in income level were associated with much smaller differences in surface area. Higher income was also associated with better performance in certain cognitive skills – cognitive differences that could be accounted for, in part, by greater brain surface area.
In the study, titled “Family Income, Parental Education and Brain Structure in Children and Adolescents,” the researchers, who were investigating the relationships between brain structure, family income, and parental education, controlled for potential differences in brain structure related to ancestral origin by collecting DNA samples from each participant.
“Family income is linked to factors such as nutrition, health care, schools, play areas and, sometimes, air quality,” said Sowell, adding that everything going on in the environment shapes the developing brain. “Future research may address the question of whether changing a child’s environment – for instance, through social policies aimed at reducing family poverty – could change the trajectory of brain development and cognition for the better.”
In fact, Noble and a team of esteemed social scientists and neuroscientists have already embarked on precisely that line of inquiry. They are in the midst of a pilot study in which mothers are given large or small monthly income payments for the first three years after their children are born. Ultimately they hope to recruit 1,000 mothers, half of whom would receive $4,000 per year ($333 per month), and half of whom would receive just $240 per year ($20 per month), a difference that is on par with the amount of the earned income tax credit.
“We’ll be looking at brain function and cognitive ability at age 3, using cognitive assessments, social and emotional development tests and brain function as measured by electroencephalography and event-related potentials,” Noble says. “If the children of mothers receiving larger payments show beneficial effects on brain function, it would be a step toward refuting the argument that poverty is a symptom, not a cause, and that wealthier parents are wealthy because they possess -- and pass on -- traits of self-discipline, determination and resilience. By identifying how income affects early child development, we hope to inform anti-poverty policies that better support children’s well-being.”Click Here for an article on Kim Noble's study by the Washington Post
For media inquiries, please contact Sara Clough at 212-678-4147 or sara.clough@tc.columbia.edu
A second story by Lyndsey Layton asks whether the study deepens the national debate about how to close the achievement gap.
An editorial by Nicholas Kristoff in The New York Times encourages a shift in attention from K-12 reform to pre-K and younger, citing the study coauthored by TC's Kim Noble and published in Nature Neuroscience, which found that children in low-income families had a brain surface area on average 6 percent smaller than that of children in high-income families.
Published Monday, Mar. 30, 2015